- Gay Christian Geek: Bad Science, Bad Hermeneutics
- Philippa Willitts at The F-Word: You're frightening me
- Zero at the Bone: Why I’m not an anthropologist
- Mindy at Hoyden About Town: Queen Victoria and now
- [trigger warning] Melissa McEwan at Shakesville: Quote of the Day
- [trigger warning] Forty Shades of Grey: Shocking News! Rapists live in the same society as we do!
- DeeLeigh at Big Fat Blog: UK Study: kids who are put into foster care tend to get heavier
- s.e. smith: ‘It’s all in your head’
- nominatissima: Courtesy & Respect
Friday, 16 December 2011
Friday Links
Wednesday, 7 December 2011
Two quick thoughts
ATOS are apparently getting rid of internal email. Apparently it's to improve productivity, which may be true, but I expect - given the dubious nature of what our government employs them to do - that the more transient nature of instant messages compared with email is a big bonus.
Meanwhile, in good news for future ATOS contracts, polling suggests an increasing number of people are blaming poverty on poor people and less willing to pay taxes to help others. Mark Easton says:
One would expect that when the economy goes down people become a little less judgmental of those in need but this year's report finds 26% of people feel poverty is the result of 'laziness' or 'lack of willpower'. In the mid-1990s that figure was down to 15%.
I'm not sure - benefit of hindsight! - one would expect that. Obviously as people get into worse financial situations, their likelihood of being in favour of extra taxes on them personally decreases. Also, of course, there's an important psychological effect of blaming poverty on the poor - if it was due to flaws in character or morals which you, of course, do not share, then it won't happen to you. It's not surprising in these times that the increased number of people who need reassurance it won't happen to them outweighs the increased number of people who know so many exceptions that they know the causes are bigger than that. So far, anyway.
Friday, 2 December 2011
Friday Links
- HarpyMarx: Workfare means exploitation
- Obesity Timebomb: Stereotyping Fat and Capitalism
- Another Angry Woman: Female orgasms and cheating risk: in which evolutionary psychology actually tests a hypothesis
- Andrew at Generalising: His and hers … cameras?
- Amadi Talks: Siri Failures, Illustrated
- echidne at Echidne of the Snakes: Autism. It's The Uppity Women's Fault.
- [trigger warning] A Bee of a Certain Age: So exactly who is at fault here?
- Lisa Wade at Sociological Images: Molson’s Divergent Marketing: Cosmo vs. Playboy
- [trigger warning] s.e. smith at Tiger Beatdown: Violence Against Women With Disabilities: Probing the Scope of the Problem
Thursday, 1 December 2011
Correlation is not causation, and other easy statistics
The local free paper's property section had an article a
reprinted press release from a property website, which noted that
odd-numbered houses were more valuable than even-numbered houses, and
that lower numbers are more valuable than higher numbers. The article
never explicitly claims that the difference in house prices is
caused by the numbering - but on the other hand the existence
of the article, and much of its wording, strongly implies that they
would like their readers to believe this.
Anyway, it makes a good uncontentious example of "correlation is not causation" (and a few other statistical lessons), so I'm going to step through their claims in detail in case I need such an example later. Yes, clichés involving aquatic creatures and round wooden containers may come to mind.
So, the figures:
House type | Average price (£) |
---|---|
Odd number | 207,202 |
Even number | 206,664 |
Number 1 | 229,411 |
Number 2 | 222,273 |
Number 13 | 203,892 |
1 to 20, except 13 | "near the top of the list" |
No number - name only | "an average 90,000 more than numbered homes" |
So, obviously - with one exception - the number of a house is not directly affecting its price. The reduction in price - by around 1-2% - for being the famously "unlucky" number 13 is understandable: many people won't buy a house numbered 13, so the number of potential buyers is lower, so the number of offers will be lower, which will (slightly) reduce the average price.
That's that one explained. What about the rest?
Firstly, note the fairly small difference between odd and even houses: around 500 pounds in 200,000 - or around 0.25%. They don't say how big the sample size was in total, but that's unlikely to be a statistically significant difference. The numbers have all been quoted to the ridiculous precision only available by taking a mean of a large sample (which lets us conclude which sort of average was in use, too) - both round to 207,000.
In so far as there is a real effect, the relatively large difference in price between 1 and 2 is likely to explain it.
So what about lower numbers being higher priced? This is a real effect, but it's not (of course) as simple as lower numbers being intrinsically more valuable. It's a combination of two factors, and their relation with the allocation algorithms for house numbers.
House numbering is most commonly either done sequentially with odd numbers on one side of the street and even on the other side, ascending in the same direction - or for less linear housing developments, sometimes as a single sequence of numbers along the street. Numbers are generally not skipped unless there's a "gap where a house could be".
So, as a consequence, on an average terraced street, house 1 (and often, but not as often, house 2) will be end-terrace houses, which are much more valuable. There will also be another set of end-terrace houses at the other end of the terrace, but there is no standardisation whatsoever in terrace lengths, so the increase in value there is scattered throughout the house numbers.
Then there's street length. In general, shorter streets have better houses. 138 Smith Street is almost certainly going to be an urban street, or maybe a main road in the suburbs with lots of traffic. Either way, not the greatest for house prices. 6 Jones Street, on the other hand, could be on a similar street, or it could equally be on a side street in an expensive rural village. So that averaging explains lower numbers being higher priced.
The final figure: un-numbered houses are significantly - over 40% - more expensive than numbered houses has much the same sort of obvious real cause. Un-numbered houses will include mansions, converted farms, actual farms, and other large isolated buildings, with asking prices potentially into the millions of pounds.
This is why "correlation is causation" is so tempting - the house numbers don't cause the price differences; position on the street and type of street do. However, position and type (plus local standard numbering algorithm) also cause the numbers - so there's a very noticeable correlation on a statistical level. Additionally, house number and house price are really easy to measure quantitatively - "type" and "numbering algorithm" are much more qualitative.
Note that the correlation is therefore no use as an individual predictor: what street a house is on will tell you far more about its price than its position on the street, and the effect is sufficient subtle that - apart from end terrace effects - it probably won't be reliably visible on a single street. This is another useful statistical reminder: a difference in averages can be virtually irrelevant if you're looking at individual cases.